Thứ Ba, 7 tháng 6, 2016

Quantitative Methods by University of Amsterdam C 2

The Scientific Method

https://www.coursera.org/learn/quantitative-methods/lecture/Dp5ip/2-01-empirical-cycle

In the first module we discussed how the scientific method developed, general philosophical approaches and the types of knowledge science aims to find. In this second module we'll make these abstract principles and concepts a little more concrete by discussing the empirical cycle and causality in more detail.
We’ll see how, and in what order these concepts are implemented when we conduct a research study. We'll also consider the main criteria for evaluating the methodological quality of a research study: Validity and reliability. The focus will be on internal validity and how internal validity can be threatened.

What would be your 'recipe' for the scientific method?

The scientific method leaves room for creativity when it comes to forming research questions; but once we have formulated a research question we need to test it methodically. The empirical cycle provides a general framework for combining systematic observation and logic to test our research questions. As you watch the video, ask yourself which principles or concepts you recognize from the videos in the first module.

2.01 Empirical Cycle



The observations collected in the testing phase can serve as new specific observations in the observation phase. This is why the process is described as a cycle. New empirical data obtained in the testing phase give rise to new insights that lead to a new run through. And that’s what empirical science comes down to. We try to hone in on the best hypotheses and build our understanding of the world as we go through the cycle, again and again. 


What will it take for you to accept a hypothesis?


The empirical cycle describes how we transform an observation into a hypothesis, that is in turn, transformed into a prediction by specifying a research setup. So far, so good. But what does it mean if our prediction is confirmed? What if it's disconfirmed? What does this mean for our hypothesis: Do we accept it or do we reject it? These questions were only briefly addressed in the previous video, so we will take a closer look at them in the following video.



2.02 (Dis)confirmation

What do you look for in a good research study?

If our predictions are confirmed we can't automatically conclude our hypothesis is supported. Alternatively, if our predictions are refuted, we don't necessarily reject our hypothesis. So how do we decide whether our results provide strong or weak support for our hypothesis? In the next video we'll discuss the general criteria to evaluate the methodological quality of a study. We will return to these criteria in much more detail later on.

2.03 Criteria


How do you identify what caused an effect?

Causality is a very important concept in relation to internal validity. So before we consider internal validity in more detail, we'll first have a look at causality. When do we consider a relation to be causal? What is required? Try to answer this question before watching the video and see if your answers match up to the criteria listed in the video. Causality can be a controversial topic, we are very interested to hear what you think in the forums!

5.03 Probability Sampling


5.04 Probability Sampling - Simple


5.04 Probability Sampling - Simple



5.05 Probability Sampling - Complex



5.06 Non-Probability Sampling



To what extent does a sample reflect the population?


Inherent in the concept of sampling is that a sample provides an incomplete picture of the population. We expect scores on the dependent variable in any particular sample to differ from the scores in the population, simply because the sample is a small subset of this population. This error is referred to as sampling error. Of course it would be useful to know, on average, how large this error is, or in other words, how precise and accurate our sample is in providing an estimate of the population values. This difficult concept will be discussed in the video on sampling error. Of course there are also other sources of error, both random and systematic. These errors, including potential biases, are referred to as non-sampling error and are discussed in a separate video.

5.07 Sampling Error




5.08 Non-Sampling Error


How large should your sample be?


The final question a researcher needs to answer - once the population is identified and a sampling method is chosen - is how many participants should be recruited. In principle more is always better, but there is a point of diminishing returns, where the cost of adding participants outweighs the benefit of higher precision. For probability sampling it is possible to calculate how large the sample should be given certain criteria for precision. If you found the concepts margin of error difficult, it is a good idea to re-watch the video on sampling error before watching this last video on sampling size!

5.09 Sample Size


Không có nhận xét nào:

Đăng nhận xét

Tìm kiếm Blog này

Lưu trữ Blog